-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 1
/
Pasturel_etal2020_PLoS-CB_SI.tex
2668 lines (2602 loc) · 166 KB
/
Pasturel_etal2020_PLoS-CB_SI.tex
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
890
891
892
893
894
895
896
897
898
899
900
901
902
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
910
911
912
913
914
915
916
917
918
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
939
940
941
942
943
944
945
946
947
948
949
950
951
952
953
954
955
956
957
958
959
960
961
962
963
964
965
966
967
968
969
970
971
972
973
974
975
976
977
978
979
980
981
982
983
984
985
986
987
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
995
996
997
998
999
1000
%!TeX TS-program = pdflatex
%!TeX encoding = UTF-8 Unicode
%!TeX spellcheck = en-US
%!BIB TS-program = bibtex
% -*- coding: UTF-8; -*-
% vim: set fenc=utf-8
% R0 https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/784116v1.full.pdf
%: %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% \newcommand{\AuthorA}{Chlo\'e Pasturel}
% \newcommand{\AuthorB}{Anna Montagnini}%
% \newcommand{\AuthorC}{Laurent Udo Perrinet}%
% \newcommand{\Address}{Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone (UMR 7289), Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS - Marseille, France}%
% \newcommand{\Website}{https://laurentperrinet.github.io}%
% \newcommand{\EmailC}{Laurent.Perrinet@univ-amu.fr}%
% \newcommand{\Title}{
% Humans adapt their anticipatory eye movements to the volatility of visual motion properties
% }
% \newcommand{\Acknowledgments}{This work was supported by EU Marie-Sk\l{}odowska-Curie Grant No 642961 (PACE-ITN) and by the Fondation pour le Recherche M\'edicale, under the program \textit{Equipe FRM} (DEQ20180339203/PredictEye/G Masson). Code and material on the \href{\Website/publication/pasturel-montagnini-perrinet-20}{corresponding author's website}. We thank Doctor Jean-Bernard Damasse, Guillaume S Masson and Professor Laurent Madelain for insightful discussions. }
% \newcommand{\KeyWords}{eye movements; decision making; volatility; Bayesian model; adaptation; perception}
%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
\documentclass[10pt,letterpaper]{article}
% \usepackage[top=0.85in,left=2.75in,footskip=0.75in]{geometry}
\usepackage{geometry}
\usepackage{amsmath,amssymb}
% Use adjustwidth environment to exceed column width (see example table in text)
\usepackage{changepage}
% Use Unicode characters when possible
\usepackage[utf8x]{inputenc}
% textcomp package and marvosym package for additional characters
\usepackage{textcomp,marvosym}
% cite package, to clean up citations in the main text. Do not remove.
% \usepackage{cite}
\usepackage{natbib}
% Use nameref to cite supporting information files (see Supporting Information section for more info)
\usepackage{nameref}
% line numbers
% \usepackage[right]{lineno}
% ligatures disabled
\usepackage{microtype}
\DisableLigatures[f]{encoding = *, family = * }
% color can be used to apply background shading to table cells only
\usepackage[table]{xcolor}
% array package and thick rules for tables
\usepackage{array}
% create "+" rule type for thick vertical lines
% \newcolumntype{+}{!{\vrule width 2pt}}
% % create \thickcline for thick horizontal lines of variable length
% \newlength\savedwidth
% \newcommand\thickcline[1]{%
% \noalign{\global\savedwidth\arrayrulewidth\global\arrayrulewidth 2pt}%
% \cline{#1}%
% \noalign{\vskip\arrayrulewidth}%
% \noalign{\global\arrayrulewidth\savedwidth}%
% }
% % \thickhline command for thick horizontal lines that span the table
% \newcommand\thickhline{\noalign{\global\savedwidth\arrayrulewidth\global\arrayrulewidth 2pt}%
% \hline
% \noalign{\global\arrayrulewidth\savedwidth}}
% % Remove comment for double spacing
% %\usepackage{setspace}
% %\doublespacing
% % Text layout
% \raggedright
% \setlength{\parindent}{0.5cm}
% \textwidth 5.25in
% \textheight 8.75in
% Bold the 'Figure #' in the caption and separate it from the title/caption with a period
% Captions will be left justified
\usepackage[aboveskip=1pt,labelfont=bf,labelsep=period,justification=raggedright,singlelinecheck=off]{caption}
\renewcommand{\figurename}{Fig}
% Use the PLoS provided BiBTeX style
% \bibliographystyle{plos2015}
% Remove brackets from numbering in List of References
\makeatletter
\renewcommand{\@biblabel}[1]{\quad#1.}
\makeatother
% Header and Footer with logo
\usepackage{lastpage,fancyhdr,graphicx}
\usepackage{epstopdf}
%\pagestyle{myheadings}
% \pagestyle{fancy}
% \fancyhf{}
% %\setlength{\headheight}{27.023pt}
% %\lhead{\includegraphics[width=2.0in]{PLOS-submission.eps}}
% \rfoot{\thepage/\pageref{LastPage}}
% \renewcommand{\headrulewidth}{0pt}
% \renewcommand{\footrule}{\hrule height 2pt \vspace{2mm}}
% \fancyheadoffset[L]{2.25in}
% \fancyfootoffset[L]{2.25in}
% \lfoot{\today}
%% END MACROS SECTION
\newcommand{\KL}[2]{\text{KL}( #1 | #2 )}
%% parenthesis
\newcommand{\pa}[1]{\left( #1 \right)}
\newcommand{\bpa}[1]{\big( #1 \big)}
\newcommand{\choice}[1]{ %
\left\{ %
\begin{array}{l} #1 \end{array} %
\right. }
% ensembles
\newcommand{\ens}[1]{ \{ #1 \} }
\newcommand{\enscond}[2]{ \left\{ #1 \;;\; #2 \right\} }
% egal par définition
\newcommand{\eqdef}{\ensuremath{\stackrel{\mbox{\upshape\tiny def.}}{=}}}
\newcommand{\eqset}{\ensuremath{\stackrel{\mbox{\upshape\tiny set}}{=}}}
\newcommand{\eq}[1]{\begin{equation*}#1\end{equation*}}
\newcommand{\eql}[1]{\begin{equation}#1\end{equation}}
\newcommand{\eqs}[1]{\begin{align*}#1\end{align*}}
\newcommand{\eqa}[1]{\begin{align}#1\end{align}}
\DeclareMathOperator{\argmin}{argmin}
\DeclareMathOperator{\argmax}{argmax}
\newcommand{\uargmin}[1]{\underset{#1}{\argmin}\;}
\newcommand{\uargmax}[1]{\underset{#1}{\argmax}\;}
\newcommand{\umin}[1]{\underset{#1}{\min}\;}
\newcommand{\umax}[1]{\underset{#1}{\max}\;}
\newcommand{\usup}[1]{\underset{#1}{\sup}\;}
% for units
\usepackage{siunitx}%
\newcommand{\ms}{\si{\milli\second}}%
%% Symboles arrondis
\newcommand{\Aa}{\mathcal{A}}
\newcommand{\Bb}{\mathcal{B}}
\newcommand{\Cc}{\mathcal{C}}
\newcommand{\Dd}{\mathcal{D}}
\newcommand{\Ee}{\mathcal{E}}
\newcommand{\Ff}{\mathcal{F}}
\newcommand{\Gg}{\mathcal{G}}
\newcommand{\Hh}{\mathcal{H}}
\newcommand{\Ii}{\mathcal{I}}
\newcommand{\Jj}{\mathcal{J}}
\newcommand{\Kk}{\mathcal{K}}
\newcommand{\Ll}{\mathcal{L}}
\newcommand{\Mm}{\mathcal{M}}
\newcommand{\Nn}{\mathcal{N}}
\newcommand{\Oo}{\mathcal{O}}
\newcommand{\Pp}{\mathcal{P}}
\newcommand{\Qq}{\mathcal{Q}}
\newcommand{\Rr}{\mathcal{R}}
\newcommand{\Ss}{\mathcal{S}}
\newcommand{\Tt}{\mathcal{T}}
\newcommand{\Uu}{\mathcal{U}}
\newcommand{\Vv}{\mathcal{V}}
\newcommand{\Ww}{\mathcal{W}}
\newcommand{\Xx}{\mathcal{X}}
\newcommand{\Yy}{\mathcal{Y}}
\newcommand{\Zz}{\mathcal{Z}}
\usepackage{gensymb} % \degree
%% ======== polices de caracteres =============
\usepackage[T1]{fontenc}%
\usepackage{lmodern}%
\usepackage{t1enc}
\usepackage{ragged2e}
%============ graphics ===================
\usepackage{graphicx}%
\DeclareGraphicsExtensions{.pdf,.png,.jpg}%
\graphicspath{{./figures/}}%
%============ bibliography ===================
%\newcommand{\citep}[1]{\cite{#1}}
%\newcommand{\citet}[1]{\cite{#1}}
%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
%% OPTIONAL MACRO FILES
\usepackage{tikz}
\usetikzlibrary{external}
\tikzexternalize[prefix=figures_eps/]
\newcommand{\seeFig}[1]{Figure~\ref{fig:#1}}
\newcommand{\seeEq}[1]{Equation~\ref{eq:#1}}
\newcommand{\seeApp}[1]{Appendix~\ref{app:#1}}
\newcommand{\seeSec}[1]{Section~\ref{sec:#1}}
%============ hyperref ===================
\usepackage[unicode,linkcolor=red,citecolor=red,filecolor=red,urlcolor=red,pdfborder={0 0 0}]{hyperref}%
%\hypersetup{%
%pdftitle={\Title},%
%pdfauthor={\AuthorA }%\ < \Email > \Address},%
%}%
\usepackage{color}%
\newcommand{\LP}[1]{\textbf{\textcolor{red}{[LP: #1]}}}
\newcommand{\Rone}[1]{\textbf{\textcolor{magenta}{[rev 1: #1]}}}
\newcommand{\AM}[1]{\textbf{\textcolor{blue}{[AM: #1]}}}
\newcommand{\CP}[1]{\textbf{\textcolor{green}{[CP: #1]}}}
\usepackage{listings}
\definecolor{dkgreen}{rgb}{0,0.6,0}
\definecolor{gray}{rgb}{0.5,0.5,0.5}
\definecolor{mauve}{rgb}{0.58,0,0.82}
%============ code ===================
\usepackage{listings}
\lstset{frame=tb,
language=Python,
aboveskip=3mm,
belowskip=3mm,
showstringspaces=false,
columns=flexible,
basicstyle={\small\ttfamily},
numbers=none,
numberstyle=\tiny\color{gray},
keywordstyle=\color{blue},
commentstyle=\color{dkgreen},
stringstyle=\color{mauve},
breaklines=true,
breakatwhitespace=true,
tabsize=3
}
%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
\title{Supporting information to ``Humans adapt their anticipatory eye movements to the volatility of visual motion properties''}%
\author{
Chlo\'e Pasturel\thanks{Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone (UMR 7289), Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS - Marseille, France},
Anna Montagnini\footnotemark[1],
Laurent Udo Perrinet\footnotemark[1] \thanks{Corresponding author:\url{Laurent.Perrinet@univ-amu.fr}}
%\thanks{\Address}
}
\date{}
% \linenumbers
%%%%%%%%%%%% Her begynner selve dokumentet %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
\begin{document}%
\maketitle%
%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% %: Abstract
% \section*{Abstract}
% Animal behavior constantly adapts to changes, for example when the statistical properties of the environment change unexpectedly. For an agent that interacts with this volatile setting, it is important to react accurately and as quickly as possible. It has already been shown that when a random sequence of motion ramps of a visual target is biased to one direction (e.g. right or left), human observers adapt their eye movements to accurately anticipate the target's expected direction. Here, we prove that this ability extends to a volatile environment where the probability bias could change at random switching times. In addition, we also recorded the explicit prediction of the next outcome as reported by observers using a rating scale. Both results were compared to the estimates of a probabilistic agent that is optimal in relation to the assumed generative model. Compared to the classical leaky integrator model, we found a better match between our probabilistic agent and the behavioral responses, both for the anticipatory eye movements and the explicit task. Furthermore, by controlling the level of preference between exploitation and exploration in the model, we were able to fit for each individual's experimental dataset the most likely level of volatility and analyze inter-individual variability across participants. These results prove that in such an unstable environment, human observers can still represent an internal belief about the environmental contingencies, and use this representation both for sensory-motor control and for explicit judgments. This work offers an innovative approach to more generically test the diversity of human cognitive abilities in uncertain and dynamic environments.
% % Please keep the Author Summary between 150 and 200 words
% % Use first person. PLOS ONE authors please skip this step.
% % Author Summary not valid for PLOS ONE submissions.
% \section*{Author summary}
% Understanding how humans adapt to changing environments to make judgments or plan motor responses based on time-varying sensory information is crucial for psychology, neuroscience and artificial intelligence. Current theories for how we deal with the environment's uncertainty, that is, in response to the introduction of some randomness change, mostly rely on the behavior at equilibrium, long after after a change. Here, we show that in the more ecological case where the context switches at random times all along the experiment, an adaptation to this volatility can be performed online. In particular, we show in two behavioral experiments that humans can adapt to such volatility at the early sensorimotor level, through their anticipatory eye movements, but also at a higher cognitive level, through explicit ratings. Our results suggest that humans (and future artificial systems) can use much richer adaptive strategies than previously assumed.
% %: %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% \section*{Motivation}
% %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% \label{sec:intro}
% %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% \subsection*{Volatility of sensory contingencies and the adaptation of cognitive systems}
% %: 1A : cognitive adaptation to volatility; general volatility and perceptual learning
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% We live in a fundamentally volatile world for which
% our cognitive system has to constantly adapt.
% In particular, this volatility may be generated
% by processes with different time scales.
% Imagine for instance you are a general practitioner and
% that you usually report an average number of
% three persons infected by flu per week.
% However, this rate is variable and
% over the past week you observe that the rate increased to ten cases.
% In consequence, two alternative interpretations are available:
% the first possibility is that there is an outbreak of flu and
% one should then estimate its incidence
% (i.e. the rate of new cases)
% since the inferred outbreak's onset, in order
% to quantify the infection rate specific to this outbreak,
% but also to update the value of %the environmental volatility (as given by
% the probability of a new outbreak %)
% at a longer time scale.
% Alternatively, these cases are
% ``unlucky'' coincidences that originate from the natural variability
% of the underlying statistical process which drive patients to the doctor,
% and which are instances drawn from the same stationary random process.
% In that option, it may be possible to readjust
% the estimated baseline rate of infection with this new data.
% This example illustrates one fundamental problem
% with which our cognitive system is faced:
% when observing new sensory evidence,
% \emph{should I stay} and continue to exploit this novel data
% with respect to my current beliefs about the environment's state
% or \emph{should I go} and explore a new hypothesis
% about the random process generating the observations
% since the detection of a switch in the environment?
%
% This uncertainty in the environment's state is characterized by its \emph{volatility}
% which by definition measures the temporal variability
% of the sufficient parameters of a random variable.
% Such meta-analysis of the environment's statistical properties
% is an effective strategy at a large temporal scale level,
% % \Rone{lines 61-63 the link to the flu outbreak could be made explicit.}\LP{Done... right?}
% as that for the flu outbreak of our example,
% but also at all levels which are behaviorally relevant,
% such as contextual changes in our everyday life.
% Inferring near-future states in a dynamic environment,
% such that one can prepare to act upon them
% ahead of their occurrence ---
% or at least forming beliefs as precise as possible
% about a future environmental context ---
% is an ubiquitous challenge for cognitive systems~\citep{Barack16}.
% In the long term, how the human brain dynamically manages
% this trade-off between exploitation and exploration
% is essential to the adaptation
% of the behavior through reinforcement learning~\citep{Cohen2007}.
%
% %: Past history of sensory event integration in vision
% In controlled psychophysical experimental settings which challenge visual perception or sensorimotor associations,
% such adaptive processes have been mostly put in evidence
% by precisely analyzing the participants' behavior in a sequence of experimental trials.
% These typically highlight sequential effects
% at the time scale of several seconds to minutes
% or even hours in the case of the adaptation to a persistent sensorimotor relation.
% % \Rone{Paragraph 76-91 is rather obscure to me, it requires some more explanation. The link to adaptation and priming is probably misleading as the measure corresponds rather to a prediction of what happens next, those two effects are typically tested differently (a reduction of sensitivity and an increase in sensitivity)}
% Indeed, stimulus history of sensory events influences
% how the current stimulus is perceived~\citep{Sotiropoulos2011,Adams12,ChopinMamassian2012,FischerWhitney2014,Cicchini_PRSB_2018} and
% acted upon~\citep{WallmanFuchs1998,Carpenter1995, Maus2015,Damasse18}.
% Two qualitatively opposite effects of the stimulus history have been described:
% negative (adaptation), and positive (priming-like) effects.
% % \Rone{line 82: not clear how adaptation "favors spatial stability of the stimulus"}
% Adaptation reduces the sensitivity to recurrently presented stimuli,
% thus yielding a re-calibrated perceptual experience~\citep{Clifford2007, Webster2011, Kohn2007}.
% %\AM{I WOULD DELETE Examples of negative biases in perceptual discrimination are numerous
% %(see for instance~\citep{KanaiVerstraten2005,ChopinMamassian2012}) and
% %show that the visual system tends
% %to favor the temporal stability of the stimulus.}
% On the other hand, priming is a facilitatory effect that
% enhances the identification of repeated stimuli~\citep{Verstraten1994, Tiest2009}:
% %\AM{I WOULD SKIP THIS This type of perceptual learning leads to improvements %in discrimination
% %with long-term training on a perceptual judgment~\citep{Lu2009}.}
% in sensorimotor control,
% the same stimulus presented several times could indeed
% lead to faster and more precise responses.
% %\AM{I WOULD DELETE and,
% %% \Rone{line 86: quite obscure}
% %at the same time, lead to a suboptimal behavior
% %as the enviroment changes and the presented stimulus is this time not coherent
% %with the participant's expectations~\citep{Hyman1953, Yu2009}.
% %This process is highly dynamic especially in complex environments
% %where new contingencies can arise at every moment.}
% Interestingly, priming effects %at cognitive levels
% are sometimes paralleled by anticipatory motor responses
% which are positively correlated with the repetition of stimulus properties.
% A well-known example of this behavior
% are anticipatory smooth eye movements (aSPEM or shortly, anticipatory pursuit),
% as we will illustrate in the next section.
%
% %: Bayesian methods & role of predictive processing for this adaptive response
% Overall, the ability to take into account
% statistical regularities in the event sequence appears as a fundamental ability
% for the adaptive behavior of living species.
% Importantly, few studies have addressed the question of whether
% the estimate of such regularities is explicit,
% and whether such explicit reports of the dynamic statistical
% estimates would eventually correlate with the measures of behavioral adaptation or priming.
% Here we aim at investigating this question
% in the specific case of the processing of a target's motion direction.
% In addition, we attempt to palliate the lack of a solid modeling approach
% to best understand the computation underlying behavioral adaptation to the environment's statistics,
% and in particular how sequential effects are integrated
% within a hierarchical statistical framework.
%
% Bayesian inference offers an effective methodology
% to deal with this question.
% Indeed, these methods allow to define and quantitatively assess
% a range of hypotheses about the processing of possibly noisy information by some formal agents~\citep{Deneve1999, Diaconescu2014, Daunizeau10a}.
% A key principle in the Bayesian inference approach is
% to introduce so-called \emph{latent variables}
% which explicitly represent different hypotheses by the agent and how these may predict experimental outcomes.
% Each hypothesis defines different weights in the graph of probabilistic dependencies between variables
% (for instance between the number of patients at a practitioner and the reality of a flu pandemic).
% %In practice, a generative model is used to to define such graph and is parameterized by structural variables
% %(such as weights or non-linear gain functions)
% %which are for instance tuned by past statistics extracted from observations.
% Then, using the rules of probability calculus and knowing incoming measurements,
% one can progressively update beliefs about the latent variables,
% and eventually infer the hidden structure underlying the received inputs~\citep{Hoyer2003, Ma2014}.
% For instance, using Bayes's rule, one can combine
% the likelihood of observations given a given generative model and
% the prior on these latent variables~\citep{Jaynes2014}
% such that beliefs about latent variables
% may be represented as probabilities.
% Of particular interest for us is the possibility to
% quantitatively represent in this kind of probabilistic model
% the predictive and iterative nature of a sequence of events.
% Indeed, once the belief about latent variables
% is formed from the sensory input,
% this belief can be used to update
% the prior over future beliefs~\citep{Montagnini2007}.
% In such models, the comparison between expectations and actual data leads to
% continuous updates of the estimates of the latent variables,
% but also of the validity of the model.
% There are numerous examples of Bayesian approaches
% applied to the study of the adaptation to volatility.
% For instance, Meyniel et al~\citet{Meyniel16} simulated a hierarchical Bayesian model
% over five previously published datasets~\citep{Squires1976, Huettel2002, Kolossa2013, Cho2002, Falk1997}
% in the domain of cognitive neuroscience.
% %\AM{NOT NECESSARY HERE Their main conclusion was that
% %learning the local transition probabilities
% %was sufficient to explain the large repertoire
% %of experimental effects reported in all these studies.}
% Here we focus on an extension of this approach to the study of motion processing and eye movements.
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% \subsection*{Anticipatory Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements (aSPEM)}
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% %: 1B : particular case of anticipatory pursuit
% %: adaptation to volatility in EMs : seen as an anticipation in SPEM - principle and function
% Humans are able to accurately track a moving object
% with a combination of saccades and
% Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements (for a review see~\citet{Krauzlis2008}).
% These movements allow us to align and
% stabilize the object on the fovea,
% thus enabling high-resolution visual processing.
% This process is delayed by different factors such as axonal transduction,
% neural processing latencies and the inertia of the oculomotor system~\citep{Krauzlis89}.
% When predictive information is available about target's motion,
% an anticipatory Smooth Pursuit Eye Movement (aSPEM or shortly, anticipatory pursuit)
% is generated before its appearance~\citep{Westheimer1954, Kowler1979a, Kowler1979b}
% thereby reducing visuomotor latency~\citep{PerrinetAdamasFriston2014}.
% Moreover, some experiments have demonstrated the existence
% of prediction-based smooth pursuit maintenance during
% the transient disappearance of a moving target~\citep{Badler2006,BeckerFuchs1985,OrbandeXivryMissalLefevre_JOV2012}
% and even predictive acceleration or deceleration during visual tracking~\citep{BeckerFuchs1985,CollinsBarnes2009}.
% Overall, although the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements
% is almost always driven by a visual motion signal,
% it is now clear that smooth pursuit behavior
% can be modulated at different stages by extra-retinal, predictive information even in the absence of a direct visual stimulation~\citep{Barnes2008}.
% Several functional and computational models have been proposed in the literature for the different forms of prediction-based smooth eye movements, such as zero-lag tracking of a periodic target~\citep{DallosJones1963} or pursuit maintenance during target occlusion~\citep{CollinsBarnes2009}. More recently an effort has been made to provide a more general theoretical framework, which is based on Bayesian inference and the reliability-based cue combination. Although the mapping of this theoretical framework onto neuronal functions remains to be elucidated, it has the clear advantage of generality, as for instance, it would encompass all forms of smooth pursuit behavior, including prediction-based and visually-guided tracking~\citep{Bogadhi2013,Orban2013,Deravet_JOV2018,Darlington_NatNeu2018}. Here, we present a model extending this recent theoretical effort to include the adaptivity to a volatile environment.
%
% Experience-based anticipatory pursuit behavior is remarkable
% in different aspects.
% First, its buildup is relatively fast, such that only a few trials are sufficient
% to observe the effects of specific regularities in the properties of visual motion, such as speed, timing or direction~\citep{Kowler1984,Maus2015,Deravet_JOV2018}.
% Second, it is a robust phenomenon, which has been observed on a large population of human participants and even in non-human primates (for a recent review see~\citet{Kowler_AnnRev2019}). Note also, that human participants seem to be largely unaware of this behavior (as inferred from informal questioning). Finally, this kind of behavior has proven to be exquisitely sensitive to the probabilistic properties of the sensorimotor context.
%
% %: linear relationship (talk about santos & kowler and others)
% Typically, anticipatory pursuit is observed after a temporal cue and
% before target motion onset~\citep{Kowler1979a,Kowler1979b, Kowler1984}. %~(see \seeFig{intro}-A).
% In previous studies~\citep{Montagnini2010,Damasse18},
% we have analyzed how forthcoming motion properties,
% such as target speed or direction, can be anticipated with coherently oriented eye movements. %~(see \seeFig{intro}-A).
% We have observed that the amplitude of anticipation,
% as measured by the mean anticipatory eye velocity,
% increases when the target repeatedly moves in the same direction.
% In particular, the mean anticipatory eye velocity is linearly related
% to the probability of motion's speed or direction. %~(see \seeFig{intro}-B).
% These results are coherent with findings
% by other groups~\citep{Kowler1984, Kowler1989, Heinen2005,SantosKowler2017} and
% they indicate that anticipatory pursuit behavior is potentially a useful marker
% to study the internal representation of motion expectancy, %(or Prior)
% and in particular to analyze how such expectancy
% is dynamically modulated by probabilistic contingencies in shaping oculomotor behavior.
% %
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %: FIGURE 1 fig:intro~\seeFig{intro}
% \begin{figure}%[b!]
% \centering{
% \begin{tikzpicture}%[thick,scale=1, every node/.style={scale=1} ]
% \node [anchor=north west] (imgA) at (0.000\linewidth,.600\linewidth){\includegraphics[width=0.325\linewidth]{1_A_Experiment_randomblock}};
% \node [anchor=north west] (imgB) at (0.335\linewidth,.595\linewidth){\includegraphics[width=0.350\linewidth]{1_B_protocol_recording}};
% \node [anchor=north west] (imgC) at (0.650\linewidth,.595\linewidth){\includegraphics[width=0.350\linewidth]{1_C_protocol_bet}};
% \draw [anchor=north west] (0.000\linewidth, .62\linewidth) node {$\mathsf{(A)}$};
% \draw [anchor=north west] (0.350\linewidth, .62\linewidth) node {$\mathsf{(B)}$};
% \draw [anchor=north west] (0.665\linewidth, .62\linewidth) node {$\mathsf{(C)}$};
% \end{tikzpicture}
% }
% \caption{
% \textbf{Smooth pursuit eye movements and explicit direction predictions in a volatile switching environment}
% We test the capacity of human participants to adapt to a volatile environment.
% \textit{(A)}~
% We use a 3-layered generative model of fluctuations in target directions (TD)
% that we call the Binary Switching model.
% This TD binary variable is chosen using a Bernoulli trial of a given probability bias.
% This probability bias is constant for as many trials until a switch is generated.
% At a switch, the bias is chosen at random from a given prior.
% Switches are generated in the third layer as binary events drawn from a Bernoulli trial
% with a given hazard rate (defined here as $1/40$ per trial). We show one realization of a block of $200$ trials.
% \textit{(B)}~
% The eye-movements task was an adapted version of a task developed by~\citet{Montagnini2010}.
% Each trial consisted of sequentially:
% a fixation dot (FIX, of random duration between $400$ and $800$~\ms),
% a blank screen (GAP, of fixed duration of $300$~\ms) and
% a moving ring-shaped target (TARGET, with $15~\degree/s$ velocity) which the observers were instructed to follow.
% The direction of the target (right or left) was drawn pseudo-randomly
% according to the generative model defined above.
% \textit{(C)}~In order to titrate the adaptation
% to the environmental volatility of target direction at the explicit and conscious level,
% we invited each observer to perform on a different day a new variant of the direction-biased experiment,
% where we asked participants to predict, \emph{before each trial}, %the level of confidence for
% their estimate of the forthcoming direction of the target.
% As shown in this sample screenshot,
% this was performed by moving a mouse cursor (black triangle) on a continuous rating scale
% between ``Left'', to ``Unsure'' and finally ``Right''.
% }
% \label{fig:intro}
% \end{figure}
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %: limits of the previous method
% %In order to generalize such results to more ecological conditions,
% %it is thus necessary to extend the experimental protocol of~\citet{Montagnini2010} in three aspects that will be illustrated in the next section.
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% \subsection*{Contributions}%Outline}
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% %: 1C : what is novel in our work
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% %: 1Ca how we do it : or rather why we do it this way (and not like Matthys)
% The goal of this study is to generalize the adaptive process
% observed in anticipatory pursuit~\citep{Montagnini2010,SantosKowler2017}
% to more ecological settings and
% also to broaden its scope by showing that such adaptive processes
% occur also at an explicit level.
% We already mentioned that by manipulating the probability bias for target motion direction,
% it is possible to modulate the strength (direction and mean velocity) of anticipatory pursuit.
% This suggests that probabilistic information about direction bias may be used
% to inform the internal representation of motion prediction
% for the initiation of anticipatory movements.
% However, %it is yet unclear what generative model to use
% previous studies have overlooked the importance to design a realistic generative model to dynamically manipulate the probability bias
% and generate an ecologically relevant input sequence of target directions.
% A possible confound comes from the fact that
% previous studies have used fixed-lengths sequences of trials, % (\textit{trial blocks}),
% stacked in a sequence of conditions defined by the different probability biases.
% Indeed, observers may potentially pick up
% the information on the block's length
% to predict the occurrence of a switch (a change in probability bias) during the experiment.
% Second, we observed qualitatively that following a switch,
% the amplitude of anticipatory pursuit velocity changed gradually,
% consistently with other adaptation paradigms~\citep{Fukushima1996,Kahlon1996,Souto13}.
% The estimate of the characteristic temporal parameters for this adaptation mechanism
% may become particularly challenging in a dynamic context,
% where the probabilistic contingencies vary in time in an unpredictable way.
% Finally, whether and how the information processing underlying
% the buildup of anticipatory pursuit and its dynamics is linked to
% an explicit estimate of probabilities is still largely unknown.
%
% %%%-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %: 1Cb design of the binary switching generative model
% To assess the dynamics of the adaptive processes
% which compensate for the variability within sensory sequences,
% one may generate random sequences of Target Directions (TDs)
% using a dynamic value for the probability bias $p = \text{Pr}(\text{TD is 'right'})$,
% with a parametric mechanism controlling for the volatility at each trial.
% In the Hierarchical Gaussian Filter model~\citep{Mathys11}, for instance,
% volatility is controlled as a non-linear transformation
% of a random walk (modeled itself by a Brownian motion with a given diffusion coefficient).
% % \Rone{line 192: Is it meant to be "volatility" and not "variability"?}
% Ultimately, this hierarchical model allows to generate a sequence of binary choices
% where volatility is controlled by %fluctuating the variability along a given trajectory.
% a specific random variable which fluctuates in time according to some probabilistic law.
% Such a forward probabilistic model is invertible
% using some simplifying assumptions and allows
% to extract a time-varying inference of the agent's belief about volatility~\citep{Vossel14}.
% % \Rone{line 195-197 the term trial block may lead to some confusion, as it is meant to be the sequence of trials with the same probability but also commonly the number of trials in a full sequence.}
% Herein, to explicitly analyze the effect of history length,
% we rather extend the protocol of~\citet{Montagnini2010} such that the probability bias
% is still fixed within sub-blocks but that these sub-blocks have variable lengths,
% that is, by introducing switches occurring at random times.
% Therefore, similarly to~\citet{Meyniel13}, we use a model for which
% the bias $p$ in target direction varies according to a piecewise-constant function.
% We expect that within each of these sub-blocks that we defined,
% the uncertainty about of the value of $p$
% will progressively decrease as we accumulate trials.
% In addition, the range of possible biases was finite ($p \in \{0, .1, .25, .5, .75, .9, 1\}$) in our previous study.
% In the present work, we also extend the paradigm
% by drawing $p$ as a continuous random variable
% within the whole range of possible probability biases (that is, the segment $[ 0, 1 ]$).
%
% %: 1Cc equations
% As a summary, we first draw random events (that we denote as ``switches'')
% with a given mean frequency (the ``hazard rate'') and which controls the strength of the volatility.
% Second, the value $p$ of the bias only changes at the moment of a switch,
% independently of the previous bias' value
% and it is stationary between two switches, forming what we call an ``epoch''.
% Third, target direction is drawn as a Bernoulli trial using the current value of $p$.
% Such a hierarchical structure is presented in~\seeFig{intro}-A,
% where we show the realization of the target's directions sequence,
% the trajectory of the underlying probability bias (hidden to the observer), and
% the occurrences of switches.
% Mathematically, this can be considered as a three-layered hierarchical model
% defining the evolution of the model at each trial $t$ as the vector $(x_2^t, x_1^t, x_0^t)$.
% At the topmost layer,
% the occurrence $x_2^t \in \{ 0, 1 \}$ of a switch ($1$ for true, $0$ for false)
% is drawn from a Bernoulli trial $\Bb$ parameterized by its hazard rate $h$
% (as the frequency of occurrence for each trial).
% The value of $\tau=\frac 1 h$ thus gives the average duration (in number of trials)
% between the occurrence of two switches.
% In the middle layer, the probability bias $p$ of target direction
% is a random variable that we define as $x_1^t \in [0, 1]$.
% It is chosen at random from a prior distribution $\Pp$
% %(that will be described in more detail in the following sections)
% at the moment of a switch,
% and else it is constant until the next occurrence of a switch.
% The prior distribution $\Pp$ can be for instance
% the uniform distribution $\Uu$ on $ [ 0, 1 ] $ or
% Jeffrey's prior $\Jj$~(see \seeApp{bcp}).
% Finally, a target moves either to the left or to the right,
% and we denote this variable (target direction, TD) as $x_0^t \in \{ 0, 1 \}$.
% This direction is drawn from a Bernoulli trial
% parameterized by the direction bias $p=x_1^t$.
% In short, this is described according to the following equations:
% %\begin{itemize}
% % \item Occurrence of a switch: $x_2^t \propto \Bb(h)$
% % \item Dynamics of probability bias: \eql{\choice{\text{if} \quad x_2^t=0 \quad \text{then} \quad x_1^t = x_1^{t-1} \\
% %\text{else} \quad x_1^t \propto \Pp }\label{eq:bsm}}
% % \item Sequence of directions: $x_0^t \propto \Bb(x_1^t)$
% %\end{itemize}
% \eql{\choice{
% \text{Occurrence of a switch: } x_2^t \propto \Bb(1/\tau) \\
% % TODO: nest the choice
% \text{Dynamics of probability bias $p=x_1^t$ }
% \choice{\text{if} \quad x_2^t=0 \quad \text{then} \quad x_1^t = x_1^{t-1} \\
% \text{else} \quad x_1^t \propto \Pp \\
% } \\
% \text{Sequence of directions: } x_0^t \propto \Bb(x_1^t)
% }\label{eq:bsm}}
% In this study, we generated a sequence of $600$ trials,
% and there is by construction a switch at $t=0$ (that is, $x_2^0=1$).
% In addition, we imposed in our sequence that a switch
% occurs after trial numbers $200$ and $400$,
% in order to be able to compare adaptation properties
% across these three different trial blocks.
% %The model generating the experimental sequence of trial directions, as well as the experimental protocol are illustrated in~\seeFig{intro}-A.
% With such a three-layered structure, the model generates the randomized occurrence of switches,
% itself generating epochs with constant direction probability %between two switches separated by a random length
% %and chosen in the continuous range of possible biases' values,
% and finally the random sequence of Target Direction (TD) occurrences at each trial.
% This system of three equations defined in~\seeEq{bsm}
% defines the Binary Switching model
% which we used for the generation of experimental sequences presented to human participants in the experiments.
% We will use that generative model as the basis for an ideal observer model equipped to
% invert that generative model in order to estimate the time-varying probability bias for a given sequence of observations (TDs).
% The comparison of human behavior with the ideal observer model's predictions will allow us to test it as a model for the adaptation of human behavior to the environment's volatility.
%
% %: 1Cd outline
% This paper is organized in five parts.
% After this introduction where we presented the motivation for this study,
% the next section~(\seeSec{Bayesian_change_point}) will present
% an inversion of the (forward) binary switching generative model,
% coined the Binary Bayesian Change-Point (BBCP) model.
% To our knowledge, such algorithm was not yet available, and
% we will here provide with an exact analytical solution
% by extending previous results from~\citet{AdamsMackay2007}
% to the binary nature of data in the Binary Switching model presented above (see~\seeEq{bsm}).
% In addition, the proposed algorithm is biologically realistic
% as it uses simple computations and is \emph{online},
% that is, all computations on the sequence may be done
% using solely a set of variables available at the present trial,
% compactly representing all the sequence history seen in previous trials.
% We will also provide a computational implementation
% and a quantitative evaluation of this algorithm.
% Then, we will present in~\seeSec{results_psycho} the analysis of experimental evidence
% to validate the generalization of previous results %.
% with this novel switching protocol.
% In order to understand the nature of
% the representation of motion regularities underlying adaptive behavior,
% we collected both
% the recording of eye movements
% and the verbal explicit judgments about expectations on motion direction.
% In one session, participants were asked to estimate
% ``how much they are confident that
% the target will move to the right or left in the next trial'' and
% to adjust the cursor's position on the screen accordingly~(see \seeFig{intro}-C).
% In the other experimental session on a different day,
% we showed the same sequence of target directions and
% recorded participants' eye movements~(see \seeFig{intro}-B).
% Combining these theoretical and experimental results,
% a novelty of our approach is to use the BBCP agent as a regressor
% which will allow us to match experimental results
% and to compare its predictive power compared to classical models such as the leaky integrator model.
% Hence, we will show that behavioral results match best with the BBCP model.
% In~\seeSec{inter}, we will synthesize these results
% by inferring the volatility parameters inherent to the models
% by best-fitting it to each each individual participant.
% This will allow the analysis of inter-individual behavioral responses for each session.
% In particular, we will test if one could extract observers' prior (preferred) volatility,
% that is, a measure of the dynamic compromise between
% exploitation (``should I stay?'') and exploration (``should I go?'')
% for the two different sessions challenging predictive adaptive processes
% both at the implicit and explicit levels.
% Finally, we will summarize and conclude this study and
% offer some perspectives for future work in~\seeSec{outro}.
% %
% %: %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% \section*{Results: Binary Bayesian Change-Point (BBCP) detection model}
% %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% \label{sec:Bayesian_change_point}
% %%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
% %
% %: 2 short intro
% %
% As we saw above, Bayesian methods provide a powerful framework for studying human behavior and adaptive processes in particular.
% For instance,~\citet{Mathys11} first defined a multi-layered generative model for
% sequences of input stimuli.
% By inverting this stochastic forward process,
% they could extract relevant descriptors at the different levels of the model
% and fit these parameters with the recorded behavior.
% Here, we use a similar approach, focusing specifically on the binary switching generative model,
% as defined in~\seeEq{bsm}.
% To begin, we define as a control a first ideal observer, the \textit{leaky integrator} (or \textit{forgetful agent}),
% which has an exponentially-decaying memory for the events that occurred in the past trials.
% This agent can equivalently be described as one
% which assumes that volatility is stationary with a fixed characteristic frequency of switches.
% Then, we extend this model to an agent
% which assumes the existence of (randomly occurring) switches, that is,
% that the agent is equipped with the prior knowledge
% that the value of the probability bias may change
% at specific (yet randomly drawn) trials,
% as defined by the forward probabilistic model in~\seeEq{bsm}.
% %
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% \subsection*{Forgetful agent (Leaky integrator) detection model}%
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% %: 2Aa justification from previous studies
% The leaky integrator ideal observer represents a classical, widespread and
% realistic model of how trial-history shapes
% adaptive processes in human behavior~\citet{Anderson2006}.
% It is also well adapted to model motion expectation in the direction-biased experiment which leads to anticipatory pursuit.
% In this model, given the sequence of observations $x_0^t$ from trial $0$ to $t$,
% the expectation $p=\hat{x}_1^{t+1}$ of the probability for the next trial direction can be modeled by making a simple heuristic~\citep{Anderson2006}:
% This probability is the weighted average of
% the previously predicted probability, $\hat{x}_1^{t}$, with the new information $x_0^t$,
% where the weight corresponds to a leak term (or discount)
% equal to $(1 - h)$, with $h \in [0, 1]$.
% At trial $t$, this model can be expressed with the following equation:
% \eql{
% \hat{x}_1^{t+1} = (1 - h) \cdot \hat{x}_1^{t} + h \cdot x_0^t
% \label{eq:leaky}}
% where $\hat{x}_1^{t=0}$ is equal to some prior value ($0.5$ in the unbiased case),
% corresponding to the best guess at $t=0$ (prior to the observation of any data).
% % NOTE: it's an AR(1) process https://stats.stackexchange.com/questions/358162/writing-ar1-as-a-ma-infty-process
%
% %: from heuristics to ideal observer
% In other words, the predicted probability $\hat{x}_1^{t+1}$ is computed
% from the integration of previous instances
% with a progressive discount of past information.
% The value of the scalar $h$ represents
% a compromise between responding rapidly
% to changes in the environment ($h \approx 1$) and
% not prematurely discarding information still of value
% for slowly changing contexts ($h \approx 0$).
% For that reason, we call this scalar the hazard rate
% in the same way to that defined for the binary switching generative model presented above (see~\seeEq{bsm}).
% Moreover, one can define $\tau = 1 / h$ as
% a characteristic time (in units of number of trials)
% for the temporal integration of information.
% Looking more closely at this expression,
% the ``forgetful agent'' computed in \seeEq{leaky}
% consists of an exponentially-weighted moving average (see \seeApp{leaky}).
% It may thus be equivalently written in the form of a time-weighted average:
% \eql{
% \hat{x}_1^{t+1} = (1-h)^{t+1} \cdot \hat{x}_1^{t=0} + h \cdot \sum_{0\leq i \leq t} (1 - h)^{i} \cdot x_0^{t-i}
% \label{eq:leaky2}}
% The first term corresponds to the discounted effect of the prior value,
% which tends to $0$ as $t$ increases.
% More importantly, as $1-h < 1$, the second term corresponds to the \emph{leaky} integration of novel observations.
% Inversely, let us now assume that
% the true probability bias for direction changes randomly with a mean rate of once
% every $\tau$ trials: $Pr(x_2^t=1)=h$.
% As a consequence, the probability that the bias does not change is $Pr(x_2^t=0)=1-h$ at each trial.
% Assuming independence of these occurrences, the predicted probability $p=\hat{x}_1^{t+1}$ is thus proportional to the sum
% of the past observations weighted by the belief that the bias has not changed during $i$ trials in the past, that is, exactly as defined by the second term of the right-hand side in~\seeEq{leaky2}.
% This shows that assuming that changes occur at a constant rate ($\hat{x}_2^t=h$)
% but ignoring more precise information on the temporal occurrence of the switch,
% the optimal solution to this inference problem is the
% ideal observer defined in~\seeEq{leaky2},
% which finds an online recursive solution in~\seeEq{leaky}.
% We therefore proved here that the heuristic derived for the leaky integrator
% is an exact inversion of the two-layered generative model
% which assumes a constant epoch-duration between switches of the probability bias.
%
% %: 2Ac using \hat{p} as a regressor & limits of the leaky integrator
% The correspondence that we proved between the weighted moving average heuristic
% and the forgetful agent model as an ideal solution to that generative model leads
% us to several interim conclusions.
% First, the time series of inferred $\hat{x}_1^{t+1}$ values
% can serve as a regressor for behavioral data
% to test whether human observers follow a similar strategy.
% % \Rone{I can't make much sense of 324-326. The point that Equation 3 assumes a constant weight is also obscure to me. Doesn't a leaky-integrator amount to weighting past trials depending on trial number? This needs to be clarified.}
% In particular, the free parameter of the model ($h$),
% may be fitted to the behavioral dataset.
% Testing different hypothesis for the value of $h$ thus allows
% to infer the agents' most likely belief in the (fixed) weight decay.
% %for instance to the data shown in~\seeFig{results_intro}.
% Now, since we have defined a first generative model
% and the corresponding ideal observer (the forgetful agent),
% we next define a more complex model,
% in order to overcome some of the limits of the leaky integrator.
% Indeed, a first criticism could be that
% this model is too rigid and does not sufficiently
% account for the dynamics of contextual changes~\citep{Behrens07}
% as the weight decay corresponds to assuming \emph{a priori} a constant precision in the data sequence, contrary to more elaborate Bayesian models~\citep{Vilares2011}.
% It seems plausible that the memory size (or history length) used by the brain
% to infer any event probability can vary, and that this variation could be related
% to an estimate of environmental volatility as inferred from past data.
% The model presented in~\seeEq{leaky2} uses a constant weight
% % (decaying with the temporal distance from the current trial)
% for all trials, while the actual precision of each trial
% can be potentially evaluated and used
% for precision-weighted estimation of the probability bias.
% To address this hypothesis, our next model is inspired
% by the Bayesian Change-Point detection model~\citep{AdamsMackay2007}
% of an ideal agent
% inferring the trajectory in time of the probability bias ($x_1^t$),
% but also predicting the probability $Pr(x_2^t=1)$ of the occurrence of switches.
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% \subsection*{Binary Bayesian Change-Point (BBCP) detection model}
% % ------------------------------------------------------------------
% \label{sec:Binary_Bayesian_change_point}
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %: FIGURE 3 fig:Bayesianchangepoint \seeFig{Bayesianchangepoint}
% \begin{figure}%[b!]
% % cf 3_Results_2.ipynb
% \centering{
% \begin{tikzpicture}[thick,scale=.95]
% \node [anchor=north west] (imgA) at (0.\linewidth,.55\linewidth){\includegraphics[width=0.33
% \linewidth]{3_BCP_model}};
% \node [anchor=north west] (imgB) at (0.36\linewidth,.580\linewidth){\includegraphics[width=0.64\linewidth]{3_BCP_readouts}};
% \draw [anchor=north west] (0.000\linewidth, .62\linewidth) node {$\mathsf{(A)}$};
% \draw [anchor=north west] (0.382\linewidth, .62\linewidth) node {$\mathsf{(B)}$};
% \end{tikzpicture}
% }
% \caption{\textbf{Binary Bayesian Change-Point (BBCP) detection model.}
% ~\textit{(A)}
% This plot shows a synthesized sequence of $13$ events,
% either a leftward or rightward movement of the target (TD).
% Run-length estimates are expressed as hypotheses about the length of an epoch
% over which the probability bias was constant,
% that is, the number of trials since the last switch.
% Here, the true probability bias switched from a value of $.5$ to $.9$ at trial $7$,
% as can be seen by the trajectory of the true run-length (blue line).
% The BBCP model tries to capture the occurrences of a switch
% by inferring the probability of different possible run-lengths.
% At any new datum (trial), this defines a Hidden Markov Model
% as a graph (trellis), where % of possible run-lengths.
% edges indicate that a message is being passed
% to update each node's probability (as represented by arrows from trial $13$ to $14$).
% Black arrows denote a progression of the run-length at the next step (no switch),
% while gray lines stand for the possibility that a switch happened:
% In this case the run-length would fall back to zero.
% The probability for each node is represented by the grey scale (darker grey colors denote higher probability)
% and the distribution is shown in the inset for two representative trials: $5$ and $11$.
% Overall, this graph shows how the model integrates information to accurately identify a switch
% and produce a prediction for the next trial (e.g. for $t=14$).
% %The black \CP{Bleu} [and green] \CP{plus de courbe verte} curve respectively represent
% %the actual [and inferred] run-length of the simulated data
% %as a function of trial number.
% %In this instance, the inferred switch is delayed
% %by one trial with respect to the true switch.
% %\CP{representation des valeurs du run-length en haut a gauche pour les essais 5 (en gris) et 8 (en bleu), on peut voir que pour l'essais 5 et 8 la probabilit\'e qu'il n'y est pas eu de switch depuis le d\'ebut est la plus importante (peut \^etre prendre un autre essais plus parlant - trial 10: proba switch à l'essais 5 est plus importante (notebook3) ?)}
% ~\textit{(B)} On a longer sequence of $200$ trials,
% representative of a trial block of our experimental sequence (see~\seeFig{intro}-A), % and~\seeFig{results_raw}),
% we show %in the top plot
% the actual events which are observed by the agent (TD),
% along with the (hidden) dynamics of the true probability bias $P_{\text{true}}$ (blue line),
% the value inferred by a leaky integrator ($P_{\text{leaky}}$, orange line)
% and the results of the BBCP model
% in estimating the probability bias $P_{\text{BBCP}}$ (green line),
% along with $.05$ and $.95$ quantiles (shaded area).
% This shows that for the BBCP model,
% the accuracy of the predicted value of the probability bias
% is higher than for the leaky integrator.
% Below, we show the belief (as grayscales) for the different possible run-lengths.
% %as a function of the trial number.
% %A darker color denotes a higher probability.
% The green and orange line correspond to the mean run-length which is inferred,
% respectively, by the BBCP and leaky models:
% Note that in the BBCP, while it takes some trials to detect switches,
% they are in general correctly identified (transitions between diagonal lines) and
% that integration is thus faster than for the leaky integrator,
% as illustrated by the inferred value of the probability bias.
% }
% \label{fig:Bayesianchangepoint}
% \end{figure}
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %: 2Ba precision in our belief of \hat{p}
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% There is a crucial difference between the forgetful agent presented above
% %\AM{WE CAN SKIP THIS:which believes that changes occur at a constant rate ($\hat{x}_2^t=h$, see~\seeEq{leaky2})}
% and an ideal agent which would invert the (generative) Binary Switching model (see~\seeEq{bsm}).
% Indeed, at any trial during the experiment,
% the agent may infer beliefs about the probability of the volatility $x_2^t$
% which itself is driving the trajectory of the probability bias $x_1^t$.
% Knowing that the latter is piece-wise constant,
% an agent may have a belief over the number of trials since the last switch.
% This number, that is called the \emph{run-length} $r^t$~\citep{AdamsMackay2007}, is useful in two manners.
% First, it allows the agent to restrict the prediction $\hat{x}_1^{t+1}$ of $x_1^{t+1}$
% only based on those samples produced since the last switch, from $t-r^t$ until $t$.
% % and which we denote as $x_0^{(r^t)}=x_0^{r^t:t}$ .
% Indeed, the samples $x_0^t$ which occurred before the last switch
% were drawn independently from the present true value $x_1^t$
% and thus cannot help estimating the latter.
% As a consequence, the run-length is a latent variable
% that captures at any given trial all the hypotheses that may be occurring.
% Second, it is known that for this estimate, the precision
% (that is, the inverse of variance) on the estimate $\hat{x}_1^{t+1}$
% grows linearly with the number of samples:
% The longer the run-length, the sharper the corresponding (probabilistic) belief.
% We have designed an agent inverting the binary switching generative model by extending
% the Bayesian Change-Point (BCP) detection model~\citep{AdamsMackay2007}.
% The latter model defines the agent as an inversion of a switching generative model
% for which the observed data (input) is Gaussian.
% We present here an exact solution for the case of the Binary Switching model,
% that is, for which the input is binary (here, left or right). %
%
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %: 2Bb prediction / update cycle
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% In order to define in all generality the change-point (switch) detection model,
% we will initially describe the fundamental steps leading to its construction,
% while providing the full algorithmic details in~\seeApp{bcp}.
% % by~\seeEq{run_length} (more details on this derivation in~\seeApp{bcp})
% The goal of predictive processing at trial $t$
% is to infer the probability $Pr(x_0^{t+1} | x_0^{0:t})$ of the next datum
% knowing what has been observed until that trial
% (that we denote by $x_0^{0:t} = \{ x_0^0, \ldots, x_0^t \}$).
% This prediction uses the agent's prior knowledge
% that data is the output of a given (stochastic) generative model (here, the Binary Switching model).
% To derive a Bayesian predictive model, we introduce
% the run-length as a latent variable which gives to the agent the possibility to represent
% different hypotheses about the input.
% We therefore draw a computational graph (see \seeFig{Bayesianchangepoint}-A) where, at any trial,
% an hypothesis is formed on as many ``nodes'' than there are run-lengths.
% Note that run-lengths may be limited by the total number of trials $t$.
% %
% %This is for instance the task that we defined
% %for the bet experiment, where each participant
% %was asked to report their level of confidence for the next outcome
% %for the next trial.
% %
% %Using the run-length $r^t$ as a latent variable at each trial $t$,
% %we define the distribution which represents our belief
% %for all different hypotheses on $r^t$.
% %which
% As a readout, we can use this knowledge of the predictive probability conditioned on the run-length,
% such that one can compute the marginal predictive distribution:
% \eql{
% Pr(x_0^{t+1} | x_0^{0:t}) =
% %\sum_{r^{t}} \beta^{(r)}_t \cdot Pr(x_0^t | r^{t}, x_0^{0:t-1})
% %\sum_{r^{t}} Pr(x_0^{t+1} | r^{t}, x_0^{0:t}) \cdot Pr(r^t | x_0^{0:t})
% %\sum_{r^{t}} Pr(x_0^{t+1} | x_0^{(r^{t})}) \cdot \beta^{(r)}_t
% \sum_{r^{t}\geq 0} Pr(x_0^{t+1} | r^{t}, x_0^{0:t}) \cdot \beta^{(r)}_t
% \label{eq:pred}
% }
% where $Pr(x_0^{t+1} | r^{t}, x_0^{0:t})$ is the probability of
% the Bernoulli trial modeling the outcome of a future datum $x_0^{t+1}$,
% conditioned on the run-length and
% $\beta^{(r)}_t=Pr(r^t | x_0^{0:t})$ is the probability for each possible run-length given the observed data.
% Note that we know that, at any trial, there is a single true value for this variable $r^{t}$
% and that $\beta^{(r)}_t$ thus represents the agent's inferred probability distribution over the run-length $r$.
% As a consequence, $\beta^{(r)}_t$ is scaled such that $\sum_{r \geq 0 } \beta^{(r)}_t = 1$.
% %a previous value $Pr(x_0^{t} | r^{t-1}, x_0^{0:t-1})$
% %and the likelihood associated to the new value $x_0^t$.
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %: 2Bc prediction cycle
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
% % Computing sufficient statistics
% %-------------------------------------------------------------%
%
% With these premises, we define the BBCP
% as a prediction / update cycle
% which connects nodes from the previous trial to that at the current trial.
% Indeed, we will \emph{predict} the probability
% $\beta^{(r)}_t$ at each node, knowing either an initial prior, or its value on a previous trial.
% In particular, at the occurrence of the first trial, we know for certain that there is a switch and
% initial beliefs are thus set to the values $\beta^{(0)}_0=Pr(r^t=0)=1$ and
% $\forall r>0$, $\beta^{(r)}_0=Pr(r^0=r)=0$.
% Then, at any trial $t>0$, as we observe a new datum $x_0^t$,
% we use a knowledge of $\beta^{(r)}_{t-1}$ at trial $t-1$,
% the likelihood $\pi^{(r)}_{t}=Pr(x_0^{t} | r^{t-1}, x_0^{0:t-1})$ and
% the transition probabilities defined by the generative model
% to predict the beliefs over all nodes: %
%
% \eqa{
% \beta^{(r)}_t \propto \sum_{r^{t-1} \geq 0} \beta^{(r)}_{t-1} \cdot Pr(r^t | r^{t-1}) \cdot \pi^{(r)}_{t}
% \label{eq:pred_node}
% }
% In the computational graph, % (\seeFig{Bayesianchangepoint}-A),
% \seeEq{pred_node} corresponds to a message passing from the nodes at time $t-1$
% to that at time $t$. % and formalized by the transition matrix $Pr(r^t | r^{t-1})$.
% We will now detail how to compute the transition probabilities and the likelihood.
%
% %In the second step, one can perform prediction
% %using the graph defined in \seeFig{Bayesianchangepoint}-A.
% %Now that we have the vector of likelihoods $\pi^{(r)}_t=\Ll(x_0^t | \mu^{(r)}_{t}, \nu^{(r)}_{t})$,
% %one can update probabilities and perform the next prediction for trial $t+1$.
% First, knowing that the data is generated by the Binary Switching model (see~\seeEq{bsm}),
% the run-length is either null at the moment of a switch,
% or its length (in number of trials) is incremented by $1$ if no switch occurred:
%
% \eql{\choice{
% \text{if} \quad x_2^t=1 \text{,} \quad r^t = 0\\
% \text{else} \quad x_2^t=0 \text{,} \quad r^t = r^{t-1} +1 }\label{eq:run_length}}%see~\seeEq{run_length}
% %\text{and else} \quad r^t = r^{t-1} +1 }\label{eq:run_length}}%see~\seeEq{run_length}
% This may be illustrated by a graph
% in which information will be represented at the different nodes for each trial $t$.
% %In a switching model like the BSM, the transition matrix % defined by the graph,
% This defines the transition matrix $Pr(r^t | r^{t-1})$
% as a partition in two exclusive possibilities:
% Either there was a switch or not.
% It allows us to compute the \emph{growth probability} for each run-length. % $r \geq 0$.
% On the one hand, the belief of an increment of the run-length at the next trial is: %, before observing a new datum:
%
% \eqa{
% \beta^{(r+1)}_t = \frac{1}{B} \cdot \beta^{(r)}_{t-1} \cdot \pi^{(r)}_{t} \cdot (1-h)
% \label{eq:beta_noswitch}
% }
% where $h$ is the scalar defining the hazard rate.
% On the other hand, it also allows to express the change-point probability as:
%
% \eqa{
% \beta^{(0)}_t = \frac{1}{B} \cdot \sum_{r \geq 0} \beta^{(r)}_{t-1} \cdot \pi^{(r)}_{t} \cdot h
% \label{eq:beta_switch}
% }
% with $B$ such that $\sum_{r \geq 0} \beta^{(r)}_{t} = 1$.
% Note that $\beta^{(0)}_t=h$ and thus $B=\sum_{r \geq 0} \beta^{(r)}_{t-1} \cdot \pi^{(r)}_{t}$.